Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 14 de 14
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Aggress Behav ; 44(1): 5-17, 2018 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28635021

RESUMO

Objectification involves reducing someone to a sexual object, rather than seeing them as a full person. Despite numerous theoretical claims that people are more aggressive toward the objectified, and empirical evidence that objectification is linked to high willingness to aggress, rape proclivity, and aggressive attitudes, no research has examined a causal link between objectification and physical aggression, particularly in the context of provocation. In two experiments, we examined this predicted link. In Experiment 1, using a 2 (objectification: no/yes) × 2 (provocation: no/yes) factorial between-subjects design, we investigated the effects of objectification, induced via body focus during a face-to-face interaction, and provocation on physical aggression toward a female confederate. Our results revealed a significant main effect of provocation, a marginal main effect of objectification, and a significant interaction between these variables. In the absence of a provocation, focusing on a woman's body increased aggression toward her. Experiment 2 replicated Experiment 1 using a video of a target woman instead of a face-to-face interaction. Again, our results showed a significant two-way interaction between objectification and provocation, wherein objectification increased aggression in the absence of provocation. Overall, this research indicates that objectification can lead to heightened physical aggression toward objectified women.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Comportamento Sexual/psicologia , Violência/psicologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estupro/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
2.
Aggress Behav ; 42(6): 598-604, 2016 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27028986

RESUMO

Inhibitory information can be expected to reduce triggered displaced aggression by signaling the potential for negative consequences as a result of acting aggressively. We examined how cognitive load might interfere with these aggression-reducing effects of inhibitory cues. Participants (N = 80) were randomly assigned to a condition in a 2 (cognitive load: high/low) × 2 (inhibiting cues: yes/no) between-subjects design. Following procedures in the TDA paradigm, participants received an initial provocation from the experimenter and a subsequent triggering annoyance from another individual. In the inhibitory cue condition, participants were told, before they had the opportunity to aggress, that others would learn of their aggressive responses. In the high cognitive load condition, participants rehearsed a 10-digit number while aggressing. Those in the low cognitive load condition rehearsed a three digit number. We found significant main effects of cognitive load and inhibitory cue, which were qualified by the expected load × inhibitory cue interaction. Thus, inhibitory cues reduced displaced aggression under low-cognitive load. However, when participants in the inhibitory cue condition were under cognitive load, aggression increased, suggesting that mental busyness interfered with the full use of inhibitory information. Aggr. Behav. 42:598-604, 2016. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
3.
Psicothema (Oviedo) ; 28(1): 96-101, feb. 2016. tab
Artigo em Inglês | IBECS | ID: ibc-148824

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The Displaced Aggression Questionnaire (DAQ) is an instrument that assesses personality differences in the tendency to displace aggression. This scale is composed of three factors: angry rumination (as affective dimension), revenge planning (as cognitive dimension), and general tendency to engage in displaced aggression (as a behavioral dimension). The present study examined the validity and reliability of the Spanish version of the DAQ. METHOD: The sample consisted of 429 student and non-student participants. RESULTS: The results showed good psychometric properties, and factor analyses revealed a clear three-factor structure. Further, preliminary data about associations between DAQ scores and indirect aggression and emotion regulation strategies are shown. CONCLUSIONS: The scale presents adequate evidence for potential use in a Spanish population. We discuss its utility for research on different types of aggression (e.g., domestic abuse)


ANTECEDENTES: el Cuestionario de Agresión Desplazada (CAD) es un instrumento diseñado para evaluar las diferencias individuales en la tendencia a mostrar una conducta agresiva, producto de una provocación inicial, dirigida a hacer daño hacia una persona distinta de la fuente responsable de tal provocación. El CAD está compuesto por tres dimensiones: rumiación de la ira (dimensión afectiva), planificación de venganza (dimensión cognitiva) y agresión desplazada (dimensión conductual). El objetivo del presente estudio fue la adaptación y validación al castellano del DAC. MÉTODO: el objetivo del presente estudio fue la adaptación y validación al castellano del DAC en una muestra de 429 adultos compuesta por estudiantes universitarios y población general. RESULTADOS: el DAC mostró buenas propiedades psicométricas y una estructura de tres factores idéntica a la versión original. Por otra parte, se aportaron datos de relaciones entre variables no analizadas hasta la fecha, mostrando la asociación del CAD con la agresión indirecta y diferentes estrategias de regulación emocional. CONCLUSIONES: este estudio aporta evidencia del potencial uso del CAD en población española. Se discute la utilidad de esta escala y su relación con otros tipos de comportamientos agresivos como la violencia doméstica


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adolescente , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adaptação Psicológica , Agressão/psicologia , Psicometria/métodos , Psicometria/estatística & dados numéricos , Ira/fisiologia , Violência Doméstica/prevenção & controle , Violência Doméstica/psicologia , Violência Doméstica/estatística & dados numéricos , Inquéritos e Questionários/normas , Inquéritos e Questionários , Estudantes/psicologia , Estudantes/estatística & dados numéricos , Análise de Dados/métodos
4.
Psicothema ; 28(1): 96-101, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26820431

RESUMO

BACKGROUND:  The Displaced Aggression Questionnaire (DAQ) is an instrument that assesses personality differences in the tendency to displace aggression. This scale is composed of three factors: angry rumination (as affective dimension), revenge planning (as cognitive dimension), and general tendency to engage in displaced aggression (as a behavioral dimension). The present study examined the validity and reliability of the Spanish version of the DAQ.  METHOD: The sample consisted of 429 student and non-student participants. RESULTS: The results showed good psychometric properties, and factor analyses revealed a clear three-factor structure. Further, preliminary data about associations between DAQ scores and indirect aggression and emotion regulation strategies are shown. CONCLUSIONS: The scale presents adequate evidence for potential use in a Spanish population. We discuss its utility for research on different types of aggression (e.g., domestic abuse).


Assuntos
Agressão , Psicometria , Inquéritos e Questionários , Análise Fatorial , Humanos , Idioma , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Espanha
5.
Aggress Behav ; 41(3): 242-52, 2015 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27541602

RESUMO

In non-gang populations, the degree of identification with an in-group and perceptions of out-group entitativity, the perception of an out-group as bonded or unified, are important contributors to group-based aggression or vicarious retribution. The link between these factors and group-based aggression, however, has not been examined in the context of street gangs. The current study assessed the relationship among in-group identification, perceptions of out-group entitativity, and the willingness to retaliate against members of rival groups who did not themselves attack the in-group among juvenile gang and non-gang members in London. Our results showed the predicted membership (gang/non-gang) × in-group identification × entitativity interaction. Decomposition of the three-way interaction by membership revealed a significant identification × entitativity interaction for gang, but not for non-gang members. More specifically, gang members who identify more strongly with their gang and perceived a rival group as high on entitativity were more willing to retaliate against any of them. In addition, entitativity was a significant predictor of group-based aggression after controlling for gender, in-group identification, and gang membership. Our results are consistent with socio-psychological theories of group-based aggression and support the proposal that such theories are applicable for understanding gang-related violence. Aggr. Behav. 41:242-252, 2015. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Assuntos
Comportamento do Adolescente/psicologia , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Processos Grupais , Delinquência Juvenil/psicologia , Identificação Social , Adolescente , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Londres , Masculino
6.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 40(8): 1037-1049, 2014 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24854477

RESUMO

Considerable research has shown that alcohol consumption can increase aggression and produce extremes in other social behaviors. Although most theories posit that such effects are caused by pharmacological impairment of cognitive processes, recent research indicates that exposure to alcohol-related constructs, in the absence of consumption, can produce similar effects. Here we tested the hypothesis that alcohol priming is most likely to affect aggression in the context of ambiguous provocation. Experiment 1 showed that exposure to alcohol primes increased aggressive retaliation but only when an initial provocation was ambiguous; unambiguous provocation elicited highly aggressive responses regardless of prime exposure. Experiment 2 showed that alcohol prime exposure effects are relatively short-lived and that perceptions of the provocateur's hostility mediated effects of prime exposure on aggression. These findings suggest modification and extension of existing models of alcohol-induced aggression.

7.
Aggress Behav ; 40(4): 337-44, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24452531

RESUMO

Criminal acts are sometimes described using animal metaphors. What is the impact of a violent crime being described in an animalistic versus a non-animalistic way on the subsequent retribution toward the perpetrator? In two studies, we experimentally varied animalistic descriptions of a violent crime and examined its effect on the severity of the punishment for the act. In Study 1, we showed that compared to non-animalistic descriptions, animalistic descriptions resulted in significantly harsher punishment for the perpetrator. In Study 2, we replicated this effect and further demonstrated that this harsher sentencing is explained by an increase in perceived risk of recidivism. Our findings suggest that animalistic descriptions of crimes lead to more retaliation against the perpetrator by inducing the perception that he is likely to continue engaging in violence.


Assuntos
Criminosos , Metáfora , Punição , Violência , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
8.
Sex Abuse ; 26(2): 178-203, 2014 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23698742

RESUMO

This research examined whether a government-initiated pilot project of mandatory polygraph testing would increase the disclosures made by community-supervised sexual offenders in the United Kingdom. The Offender Managers of 332 pilot polygraph sexual offenders and 303 sexual offenders who were receiving usual community supervision were telephoned quarterly, over a 21-month period, to collect information about numbers of clinically relevant disclosures, the seriousness of disclosures made, and actions taken as a result of disclosures. Perceptions of polygraph usefulness were also collected. Offender Managers in the pilot polygraph group-compared to comparison Offender Managers-reported (a) a higher proportion of offenders making at least one disclosure (i.e., 76.5% vs. 51.2% respectively), and (b) that their offenders made more total disclosures overall (Ms = 2.60 vs. 1.25 respectively). The majority of disclosures made by sexual offenders in the polygraph group were associated with the polygraph session itself. Polygraph Offender Managers reported being more likely to take an action that involved increasing supervision, informing a third party, informing Multi-Agency Public Protection Arrangements (MAPPA), changing supervision focus, or issuing a warning to the offender. However, the relative seriousness of disclosures did not appear to differ across groups. In terms of polygraph test results, one third of offenders (most notably those who were higher in risk) failed their first test with "Deception Indicated." This outcome-received on a first test-was most likely to elicit clinically relevant disclosures. Offender Managers described the polygraph as aiding supervision strategies. This research and its associated caveats are discussed.


Assuntos
Resposta Galvânica da Pele , Detecção de Mentiras , Delitos Sexuais/prevenção & controle , Revelação da Verdade , Adulto , Criminosos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Prevenção Secundária , Reino Unido
9.
Aggress Behav ; 39(1): 13-29, 2013 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23042637

RESUMO

Four studies present the first evidence showing that public (vs. private) provocation augments triggered displaced aggression by increasing the perceived intensity of the provocation. This effect is shown to be independent of face-saving motivation. Following a public or private provocation, Study 1 participants were induced to ruminate or were distracted for 20 min. They then had an opportunity to aggress against another person who either acted in a neutral or mildly annoying fashion (viz. triggering event). As expected, the magnitude of the greater displaced aggression of those who ruminated before the triggering event compared with those distracted was greater under public than private provocation. Study 2 replicated the findings of Study 1 and confirmed that public provocations are experienced as more intense. Studies 3 and 4 both manipulated provocation intensity directly to show that it mediated the moderating effect of public/private provocation found in Study 1. The greater intensity of a public provocation increases reactivity to a subsequent trigger, which in turn, augments triggered displaced aggression.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Ira , Motivação , Pensamento , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores de Tempo
10.
Aggress Behav ; 38(1): 89-97, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24833605

RESUMO

The concept of gang aggression oftentimes elicits images of brutal intergang violence. In reality, gang-related aggression can vary widely, can have various motivations and causal factors, and includes interpersonal as well as intergroup aggression. This study examined the tendency of UK youth to engage in displaced aggression (aggression aimed at undeserving targets) and examined the relationship among gang affiliation, ruminative thought, and aggression levels. Students in three London schools were asked to complete a questionnaire that assessed levels of gang affiliation, rumination about aversive events, and a tendency to engage in displaced aggression. Our analyses found a three-way interaction between gang affiliation, rumination, and gender, such that males who were high in affiliation and rumination had the greatest tendency to displace aggression toward innocent others. Additionally, it was shown that rumination could account for a significant part of the correlation between gang affiliation and displaced aggression. Furthermore, regression analyses showed that even after controlling for trait aggression, anger, hostility, and irritability, rumination remained a significant predictor of displaced aggression. The implications for understanding gang-related aggression and for conducting future research in this area were discussed.


Assuntos
Comportamento do Adolescente/psicologia , Afeto , Agressão/psicologia , Delinquência Juvenil/psicologia , Grupo Associado , Violência/psicologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Hostilidade , Humanos , Delinquência Juvenil/estatística & dados numéricos , Masculino , Problemas Sociais , Reino Unido , Violência/estatística & dados numéricos
11.
Br J Soc Psychol ; 50(Pt 2): 281-301, 2011 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21545459

RESUMO

Although rumination following a provocation can increase aggression, no research has examined the processes responsible for this phenomenon. With predictions derived from the General Aggression Model, three experiments explored the impact of two types of post-provocation rumination on the processes whereby rumination augments aggression. In Experiment 1, relative to distraction, self-focused rumination uniquely increased the accessibility of arousal cognition, whereas provocation-focused rumination uniquely amplified the accessibility of aggressive action cognition. In Experiment 2, provocation-focused rumination uniquely increased systolic blood pressure. In Experiment 3, both types of rumination increased aggressive behaviour relative to a distraction condition. Angry affect partially mediated the effects of both provocation- and self-focused rumination on aggression. Self-critical negative affect partially mediated the effect of self-focused rumination but not provocation-focused rumination. These findings suggest that provocation-focused rumination influences angry affect, aggressive action cognition, and cardiovascular arousal, whereas self-focused rumination increases self-critical negative affect, angry affect, and arousal cognition. These studies enhance our understanding of why two types of post-provocation rumination increase aggressive behaviour.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Emoções , Pensamento , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos
12.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 34(10): 1382-95, 2008 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18660383

RESUMO

Sometimes aggression is displaced onto a target who is not totally innocent but emits a mildly irritating behavior called a triggering event. In three experiments, the authors examine stable personal attributes of targets that can impact such triggered displaced aggression (TDA). Lower levels of TDA were directed to targets whose attitudes were similar as compared to dissimilar to those of the actor (Experiment 1) and to targets who were ingroup as compared to out-group members (Experiment 2). Conceptually replicating the findings of Experiments 1 and 2, the manipulated valence of the target (viz., liked, neutral, and disliked) functioned in a similar manner, with positive valence serving a buffering function against a triggering action that followed an initial provocation (Experiment 3). The results from all three experiments are consistent with cognitive neoassociationist theory.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Deslocamento Psicológico , Processos Grupais , Comportamento Social , Desejabilidade Social , Percepção Social , Atitude , Cognição , Vítimas de Crime , Feminino , Hostilidade , Humanos , Masculino , Grupo Associado
13.
Aggress Behav ; 34(1): 25-33, 2008.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17486573

RESUMO

Alcohol increases the aggression-augmenting effects of provocation. Theories of alcohol and aggression suggest that impaired cognitive processing induced by acute intoxication leads individuals to process aggression-inducing social cues differently depending on whether they are high or low in salience. We examined the effects of intoxication and aggressive cue salience within the triggered displaced aggression paradigm. An ethnically diverse sample of 74 primarily young adult participants (40 men and 34 women; M=23.28, SD=3.14 years) were recruited from the university community and surrounding area. All participants were provoked by an experimenter, randomly assigned to a 2 (alcohol condition: alcohol vs. placebo) x 2 (trigger salience: high vs. low salience) between-subjects design, and then given the opportunity to aggress against the undeserving triggering agent. As expected, intoxication combined with a salient triggering cue elicited the most displaced aggression among all conditions. These results provide the first evidence that the effect of alcohol on triggered displaced aggression is moderated by the salience of the triggering event.


Assuntos
Agressão/efeitos dos fármacos , Agressão/psicologia , Intoxicação Alcoólica/psicologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Deslocamento Psicológico , Adulto , Intoxicação Alcoólica/sangue , Intoxicação Alcoólica/diagnóstico , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Psicológicos , Valores de Referência
14.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 88(6): 969-83, 2005 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15982116

RESUMO

Ruminating about a provocation increases the likelihood of displaced aggression following a minor annoyance (trigger). In Study 1, provoked participants who ruminated for 25 min were more aggressive toward a fumbling confederate than were distracted participants. Provocation-induced negative affect was positively related to aggression but only among those who ruminated. Study 2 conceptually replicated Study 1 and also found that the more negatively people reacted to the trigger, the more likely the trigger was to increase displaced aggression. Study 3 replicated the findings of Studies 1 and 2 by using an 8-hr rumination period. All 3 studies suggest that ruminating about a provocation increases the likelihood that a minor triggering annoyance will increase displaced aggression.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Comportamento Social , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...